Colors and patterns for distraction and escape:
Animals put a lot of time and energy
trying to avoid being preyed upon, as examples they can camouflage
themselves, or trying to make themselves less interesting, or
advertise that they are distasteful or poisonous. But we all know
that there are predators in these world, they live and they thrive,
which means they must get food somehow. So what happens, if in
despite of camouflage or other strategies to avoid attack, a predator
spots something and decides to attack? Does this mean there is no
escape? Of course there might be. Everyone knows that lots of animals
have claws, teeth, and other defense mechanisms to fight of a
predator physically, and some also chemically, like a bombarding
beetle or a skunk. But did you also know that some animals rely on
special colors and patterns as a distraction maneuver? Well, here are
some ways animals can actually escape an attack without going into
physical interactions and without being dangerous.
One of the most famous strategies is
startle display(Wilson 2009). This means that an animal being
attacked, suddenly show off some powerful and strong colors or
patterns that will cause some kind of reaction in the predators
behavior. Usually the flashing of color patterns causes the predator
to hesitate for a moment, giving the prey some extra time, although
maybe just a second or two, but enough to make an escape. Imagine
that you are walking past a door, and someone is hiding behind it,
and then suddenly jumps out in front of you shouting, the person
doesn't need to be dangerous or be a treat to you at all, but you
still will display some kind of reaction like freeze for a second or
jump to the side to avoid that person. Basically, when something
happens fast, which you don't see coming, your brain needs time to
adjust and think about what happens and how you should react. This is
kind of the same thing that happens when an animal suddenly goes from
for example cryptic colors matching the background, til showing of
all sorts of colors in a second. This is likely to be extra useful
for animals that are slow or clumsy fliers, such as mantises and
stick insects, because they need time to prepare for flight(Langridge
2009)
The peacock butterfly has cryptic colors when in a resting position, but if a threat appears, it will open its wings and flash its amazing colors. From: <http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Inachis_io_bottom_side.jpg>, viewed 3 April 2014 |
From: <http://www.warrenphotographic.co.uk/26028-peacock-butterfly>, viewed 3 April 2014. |
Some patterns are proved to be
especially good when it comes to creating a hesitation, and can even
make the predator retreat. Eye spots are such patterns(Wilson 2009),
they seem to be working great, and the pattern is found among many
different animals, but is especially abundant among butterflies and
moths. These eye spots are found on the wings, and is usually not
visible when the butterfly or moth rests. But if in danger, the wings
will be spread out, and suddenly a set of 'eyes' appear. The
predator, in these cases often birds, will be very surprised and
hesitate and might also pull back for a second. Sometimes the
predator actually go for a retreat, and this is thought to be because
the spots look like the eyes of an owl. And since owls are predators,
many smaller animals then back off. Not only moths and butterflies
display this pattern, also frogs, octopuses, stick insects, mantises
and caterpillars display it, which seems to strengthen the theory of
this strategy as being a successful one. It is obviously a feature
that has convergently appeared in different groups of animals, and
usually if something like this happens, it is because it has an
advantage somehow.
This picture shows a Lo moth with eyespots similar to a big animal like an owl. Picture from: < http://lepcurious.blogspot.com.au/2011/03/defenses-of-butterflies-eyespots.html > viewed 3 April 2014 |
Another trick some animals use, is
displaying some kind of pattern or colors that look similar to a head
or at least something that confuses the predator when it comes to
which end is which(Wilson 2009). They might also conceal their real
eyes in colors or stripes that makes the eyes less visible and this
can create more confusion. Butterflies can for example have eyespots,
something that looks like antenna, and a shape that may appear as a
head on distance. The prinsiple behind this is to make the predator
attack the side that is less vulnerable, and it also makes it
possible to get away, because the predator will misjudge the
direction the prey might be heading for an escape. Lots of animals
can survive another day with a lost peace of their tail, but injuries
on their anterior side will often be fatal(if they don't get eaten).
Butterflies, fishes and centipedes are some animals that uses this
technic to confuse predators. An animal that also uses this technic
is a caterpillar that has eyespots making it look like a snakes head,
fooling a predator in to believing that it is something entirely
different than it actually is, which might scare a predator and make
it retreat.
This picture show two caterpillars that has patterns on their head making them similar in appearance to a snake. Picture from: < http://lepcurious.blogspot.com.au/2011/03/defenses-of-butterflies-eyespots.html > viewed 3 April 2014 |
This fish show a deflection display, drawing attention away from its head towards it's posterior end. Notice how the real eyes are goes almost unnoticed. Picture from: <http://www.eplantscience.com/index/general_zoology/deflecting_an_attack.php>, viewed 3 April 2014 |
Although these technics mentioned seem
to work in many cases, there are some drawbacks as well(Langridge
2009). If a predator don't use visual cues, like if the predator have
bad/no vision or don't see all colors, it might not have any effect
at all, so it comes down to which predator that actually hunts a
particular prey.
Another effect you can find, is
habituation, which means the predator is exposed to the flashing or
the pattern several times and over time learn that this doesn't mean
anything, or after a while learn to attack the real anterior end
after several times of trial and failures. They get used to the
display the prey puts up, and then the startling and frightening
effect wears off. This means that it probably works best if the
predator is a generalist.
Biography:
Wilson, C 2009, Animal behavior:
Animal Defenses, Chelsea House, Infobase Publishing, United
States.
Langridge, KV 2009, 'Cuttlefish
use startle displays, but not against large predators',
Animal Behaviour, Vol.77, No.4,
pp. 847-856, viewed 2 April 2014, <
http://www.sciencedirect.com.elibrary.jcu.edu.au/
>.
Interesting topic and great examples! I think this is an excellent strategy for animals to employ when faced by predation. The evolution of this colouration is fascinating. I wonder how specific colouration, such as eye spots, started to evolve (i.e. what was the first colouring trait that afforded an advantage from predation?) Have you come across any idea of how this trait might first have evolved?
SvarSlett